Outline and Evaluate Research into Institutional
Aggression (24 marks)
Sykes investigated prison aggression
in particular and found that innate behaviour is a response to the problems of
adjustment posed by deprivations or ‘pains’ of imprisonment. The five
deprivations were liberty, autonomy, goods/services, heterosexual relationships
and security. These deprivations cause stress and frustration in prisoners and
aggression is a response to release this stress. Moreover, this aggression also
enables a prisoner to gain control over the oppressive social order imposed
upon them. McCorkle et al (1995) conducted a large-scale study of more than 371
US state prisons to investigate the effects of environmental influences of
inmate violence rates. The variables they measured were crowding, security
level, officer-to-inmate ratio, program involvement, size of institution and
unemployment rate of the local area around the establishment. McCorkle found that the white-black guard ratio were
positively correlated to assault rates, suggesting that institutional violence
is partly a racial issue and not deprivation. However, it may be that the
prisoners are deprived of ‘respected’ prison guards, with white, racist
prisoners being controlled by black guards of whom they don’t respect and vice
versa. The study also found that poor prison
management led to individual acts of violence, proving the deprivation model-
with aggression being implemented when the prisoners are denied a stress-free
experience- and that the removing of privileges triggered outbursts of
individual violence. The more prisoners were involved with education and skills
programmes, the more the decrease in institutional violence suggesting that the
more prisoners were given things the less they were deprived and prone to
violence. Interestingly though, overcrowding
did not influence inmate-on-inmate assault rates disproving the deprivation
model and showing the study to lack internal validity. Nijman also found that
increasing personal space didn’t reduce the levels of violence among patients
in psychiatric institutions, suggesting that this factor isn’t specific enough
in its objectivity. In McCorkle’s study, the very large sample is a significant
positive of this experiment, and the real-life quality and nationalism is
another- the realism making it easier to generalise. Apart from the study’s
findings half disproving the study, the array of variables that were measured
create a rounded and comprehensive study that has accounted for the many major
factors that may influence institutional aggression. However, despite its
nationalism, it’s still culturally biased to the U.S.A and may not reflect
prison environments from around the rest of the world. It is also supposedly
androcentric, with female prisons not being mentioned and again, making it hard
to generalise.
The deprivation model has much
research support. Wilson studied Prison Woodhill in the early 90s and changed
the environment, making it less claustrophobic, played radio sound to mask the
prison sounds and reduced the too-hot temperature. These changes virtually
eradicated assaults on prison staff and inmates, proving that small
considerations and amendments on basic deprivations can improve violence rates.
This study was conducted in a period where the situational factors in prisons
would be much different to todays and therefore can’t be relied upon
completely. Gaes and Macuire’s research also found that the deprivation of
space, or overcrowding, led to prison violence.
There is also research on
unexperienced staff. Davies and Burgess found that the length of service by
prison staff affected assaults, where more experienced officers were less
likely to suffer assault. In a hospital setting, Hodgkinson found a similar
result. With trainee nurses more likely to suffer violent assaults than
experienced nurses. These studies demonstrate that the person’s deprivation of
experienced staff which, in turn, reduce the stress of the experience and make
the adjustment easier, led to prison violence. However, it could be said that
the inexperienced staff were easier to target their pent-up derivational
aggression onto. In this case, inexperienced staff are not a deprivation for
prisoners but are the easiest recipient of institutional aggression due to
deprivation.
Individual differences contend
with the deprivation model and pose a weakness. Kane and Janus suggest that
people with serious criminal convictions are more likely to be aggressive. This
suggests that the reason why individuals have been incarcerated may be the
determining factor for institutional aggression. Crimes such as fraud are
non-aggressive, and those who are in prison because of these crimes are less
likely to become aggressive rather than those who have been convicted of
assault under derivational circumstances. Poole and Regoli found that
pre-institutional violence was the best indicator of violence in a juvenile
detention centre regardless of situational factors. It could be said that these
prisoners prone to institutional violence have initially been deprived of
things (parental love, role models, attachments, basic rights and education) in
early childhood or life which has led them to be imprisoned. With this in mind,
it is only logical that these people will continue to be violent in a prison
environment, which further deprives them of basic rights and materials to be
happy. It is therefore only logical to reduce deprivation in prison to reduce
violence in institutions and outside. Bastoy Island Ecological Prison in Norway
is evidential. Run like a society, fostering respect and focusing on
reintegrating inmates into society, with staff with social work qualifications
and decent housing arrangements. The executive of this prison looks at “this
place as a place of healing, not just of your social wounds but of the wounds
inflicted on you by the state in your four or five years in eight square metres
of high security”- or in other words deprivation in early life; and deprivation
in strict institutions. This seems to have worked, with a 16% reoffending rate
compared with 70% across the rest of Europe. This does have some ethical
issues, with victims of the crime unhappy with a punishment that doesn’t sound
too unpleasant and may find it unjust but in the long term, it is positively
looked at as helping society in the long-term.
Another weakness with the
studies on the deprivation model is that it is gender-biased. Male aggression
is predominantly measured rather than female violence in institutions. Further
research would need to be conducted to see if there is a biological factor
which influences aggression. For example, males could be more prone to violence
in prisons due to biological differences to females. The research also tends to
originate in US prisons, and therefore the findings cannot be generalised to
other institutional settings around the world.
This model is deterministic as
it suggests that all people will be aggressive if they feel deprived. However,
research has shown that not every person will choose to be aggressive within an
institutional setting, suggesting that we do have free will. People respond to
deprivation in different ways. It is also reductionist as it reduces aggressive
behaviour down to feeling of deprivation- it could be said Deindividuation is
the reason for institutional violence. This reductionism therefore suggests
that people are blank-slates when they enter the institutional setting as it
ignores the role of people’s life experiences and traits as well as their
biology- some people are more chemically prone to violence than others.
However, the research into deprivation can be used to reduce aggression levels
in institutions. By improving the conditions of the situation, the levels of
aggression should reduce. This promotes wellbeing for the staff and the
patients/prisoners. The research is also adaptable to a variety of
institutional settings such as schools. This model could explain why some
children in schools are more aggressive than others in the playground.
No comments:
Post a Comment